If you’ve been tasked with teaching the Common Core math standards, you have probably been struggling with its increased conceptual focus. What does it mean to teach math conceptually? How are we supposed to teach for conceptual understanding while continuing to demand procedural fluency? How can we shift our own mindsets, and then those of our students, to see how procedures and concepts work together in math?
We don’t have the answer, but two important pieces of research might help.
First up: a 1999 study called “Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of Mathematics: Does One Lead to the Other?” by Bethany Rittle-Johnson and Martha Wagner Alibali. In this study, researchers tested 4th and 5th graders’ ability to understand, solve problems, and transfer their understanding of the concept of equivalency and its related procedures. They gave some students instruction in the procedures, and others a heavier dose of the concept.
The students were then asked to solve some basic problems, such as  “3 + 5 + 7 = 3 + __ “. After the students solved the problems, the researchers interviewed them about their conceptual understanding (What does it mean for two things to be equal? What does the equals sign mean?) and asked them to solve problems in an unfamiliar format to test transfer.
The findings? Teaching students about the concept of equivalency led to increased understanding and the ability to generate and transfer a procedure for determining equivalency. Teaching students the procedures also helped develop their conceptual understanding and ability to adopt the correct procedure. However, students who only received procedural instruction had difficulty transferring their knowledge to unfamiliar situations. For instance, they may not try to use the same procedure for the problem 3 + ___ = 3 + 5 + 7 as they would if the missing number was on the other side of the equal sign.
The key takeaway was this: Increases in procedural knowledge lead to increases in conceptual knowledge, and vice versa. However, transfer of knowledge depends much more on conceptual knowledge than it does on procedural.
Second: a study called “Re-conceptualizing Procedural Knowledge: The Emergence of “Intelligent” Performances Among Equation Solvers” by Jon Star of Michigan State University. In this study, Star gave two sets of students identical problems to solve, but then used class discussion to focus either on procedural accuracy and fluency, or conceptual understanding. The discussions were the same length, but in the procedural discussion the focus of conversation was finding the right numeric answer, following the right steps, and how to check to see if the numeric answer was correct. In the conceptual conversations, students were asked to compare different approaches to the problems and come up with alternative methods for solving the problem. The result? Students who participated in more conceptual discussions and had focused on finding alternative ways of solving the same problems were more flexible and innovative in their solution methods after the experiment.
Although neither of these studies solves the problem of balancing conceptual understanding and procedural knowledge in our math classrooms, they do point out the important influence that conceptual understanding can have on procedural transfer, flexibility, and innovation.
 

Discover more from learning that transfers

Subscribe now to keep reading and get access to the full archive.

Continue reading